DE eng

Search in the Catalogues and Directories

Page: 1...3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11...2.306
Hits 121 – 140 of 46.109

121
THE ROLE OF PSYCHOLINGUISTIC ASPECTS IN THE LEARNING AND TEACHING SECOND LANGUAGE ...
Ezoza Ismat Kizi Hazratkulova. - : Academic research in educational sciences, 2022
BASE
Show details
122
The Rise and Fall of Linguistic Transfer ...
Ozernyi, Daniil M.. - : Zenodo, 2022
BASE
Show details
123
The Rise and Fall of Linguistic Transfer ...
Ozernyi, Daniil M.. - : Zenodo, 2022
BASE
Show details
124
The contributions of phonological awareness and decoding on spelling in isiXhosa Grade 3 readers ...
Schaefer, Maxine. - : Open Science Framework, 2022
BASE
Show details
125
Learning vocabulary and syntax with and without redundancy ...
Monaghan, Padraic. - : Open Science Framework, 2022
BASE
Show details
126
Phonological Contrast and Conflict in Dutch Vowels: Neurobiological and Psycholinguistic Evidence from Children and Adults ...
Rue, N.P.W.D. De. - : Data Archiving and Networked Services (DANS), 2022
BASE
Show details
127
Rapport et Bilan Scientifique - 2e Symposium sur la Politique Linguistique Familiale 2021
In: https://hal-inalco.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-03525635 ; [Rapport de recherche] INALCO, Sorbonne Paris-Cité (SPC). 2022 (2022)
BASE
Show details
128
The Oral Production of Discourse Markers by Advanced Learners of Spanish
In: IULC Working Papers; Vol. 22 No. 1 (2022): Volume 22 (1) ; 1524-2110 (2022)
BASE
Show details
129
Stakeholders' Insights Into Migrant Students’ Experiences in a Thai Public School: A Linguistic Ecological Perspective
In: ASEAS - Austrian Journal of South-East Asian Studies ; 14 ; 2 ; 243-266 ; Multicultural Lingual and Multicultural Education (2022)
BASE
Show details
130
Evaluación y descripción del desarrollo del discurso narrativo en español
Camus Torres, Alondra; Aparici Aznar, Melina; Morgan, Gary. - : Grupo Ars XXI de Comunicacion, S.L., 2022
BASE
Show details
131
Course Progress in the General Integration Course
In: 7-2021 ; BAMF-Brief Analysis ; 13 (2022)
BASE
Show details
132
Impact of factors specific to forced migration on German language acquisition: Family constellation, health status and housing situation
In: 4-2020 ; BAMF-Brief Analysis ; 13 (2022)
BASE
Show details
133
The third wave of the IAB-BAMF-SOEP Survey of Refugees: Refugees are improving their German language skills and continue to feel welcome in Germany
In: 1-2020 ; BAMF-Brief Analysis ; 18 (2022)
BASE
Show details
134
How do L2 student moderators facilitate a peer-led discussion forum?
Zhong, Dr. Qunyan ( Maggie); Norton, Howard. - 2022
BASE
Show details
135
Introduction to Language Development in Children: Description to Detect and Prevent Language Difficulties
In: Children, 2022, vol. 9, núm. 3, p. 412 ; Articles publicats (D-PS) (2022)
BASE
Show details
136
Relative clauses in child heritage speakers of Turkish in the United States
Coşkun Kunduz, Aylin; Montrul, Silvina. - : John Benjamins Publishing Company, 2022
BASE
Show details
137
Spanish pragmatic markers' usage patterns in second language and heritage speakers
Abstract: This study examines the patterns of use of pragmatic markers in heritage speakers and second language (L2) learners of Spanish at different proficiency levels (advanced vs. low-intermediate) by comparing their performances in a series of oral tasks. The investigation is guided by two main goals: 1) to analyze whether these groups display the same patterns of pragmatic markers’ usage in terms of frequency, variety of markers and range of functions; 2) to examine how the variables of proficiency, speech rate, language use, and participation in immersion programs affect their use of pragmatic markers. Based on previous research, I hypothesized that heritage speakers would outperform L2 learners of Spanish, therefore, they would use pragmatic markers more frequently, as well as a broader range of pragmatic markers, and for a wider variety of functions in their oral discourse (Fernández et al., 2014; Montrul, 2008, 2011; Torres, 2002; Torres & Potowski, 2008; Said-Mohand, 2006; Sánchez-Muñoz, 2007). Additionally, I hypothesized the creation of an Intercultural Style (Blum Kulka, 1991; Kasper & Blum-Kulka, 1993) by the bilingual groups. This style is characterized by a) cross-linguistic transfer from the L1 to the L2 (including examples of code-switching and/or borrowings, as well as the transfer of certain discourse-pragmatic functions from the English pragmatic markers to the Spanish ones); b) the overuse of certain pragmatic markers; c) instances of uses of pragmatic markers that diverge from the “monolingual standardized norm” (Aijmer, 2011; Bou, Garcés & Gregori, Granger & Tyson, 1996; Cenoz, 2003; Gilquin, 2008; Nogueira da Silva, 2011; Said-Mohand, 2006; Sánchez-Muñoz, 2007; Sankoff et al., 1997; Thomas, 1983; Torres, 2002; Torres & Potowski, 2008). Regarding the second goal, I hypothesized that the variables of proficiency, speech rate, language use, and immersion would play a crucial role in the use of pragmatic markers for both bilingual groups. Regarding proficiency, I expected more proficient groups (advanced heritage speakers and L2 learners) to outperform their less proficient counterparts (low-intermediate) (Fernández et al., 2014; Torres, 2002; Torres & Potowski, 2008; Said-Mohand, 2006). Given the fact that naturalistic contexts and direct contact with native speakers of the target language favor the acquisition of pragmatic markers (Hellermann & Vergun, 2007; Polat, 2011; Sankoff et al., 1997), both the use of Spanish, and participation in immersion programs were also predicted to be positively correlated with the production of pragmatic expressions. A total of 77 participants took part in the experiment: 25 heritage speakers (13 advanced and 12 low-intermediate), 32 L2 learners (8 advanced and 24 low-intermediate), and 20 monolingually-raised native speakers of Spanish as a control group (10 speakers of Mexican Spanish and 10 of the Peninsular variety). Participants completed an oral task designed to elicit oral speech samples via a computer. Prompts included different contexts with open-ended questions (e.g., describe your best friend) and more interactive contexts (e.g., the performance of speech acts such as apologizing to a friend). A total of 539 oral samples (77 participants x 7 prompts) were transcribed and coded for pragmatic markers. Results from statistical analyses (one-way between-subjects ANOVA and Tukey HSD post-hoc analyses conducted in R) showed that all bilingual groups produced similar rates of pragmatic markers. In terms of the variety of expressions employed by participants, statistical analyses revealed significant differences between the low-intermediate L2 group and the rest of the bilingual groups, as well as between the advanced L2 learners and the low-intermediate HSs. No significant differences were found, however, between both heritage speakers’ groups nor between the heritage speakers and the advanced L2 group. Concerning the most frequent pragmatic expressions, there were similar patterns across all groups. For instance, ‘y’, ‘pero’, and ‘como’ were among the five most frequent pragmatic markers for all groups (including monolingually-raised native speakers). On the other hand, there were also differences between L2 learners and heritage speakers. For example, heritage speakers and monolinguals favored the use of ‘este’ and ‘pues’, whereas L2 learners preferred the use of ‘entonces’ and ‘sí’. The oral speech of both heritage speakers and L2 learners showed features that differentiated them from the monolingually-raised groups. Their Intercultural Style was characterized by the use of English pragmatic markers (e.g., ‘so’, ‘like’); higher frequencies of use of markers such as ‘como’, ‘también’, or ‘sí’; the overuse of certain markers in the speech of low-intermediate L2 learners (e.g., ‘y’, ‘pero’); as well as uses of pragmatic markers in ways that diverged from the “norm” of monolingual varieties (e.g., ‘también no’, ‘unfortunadamente’, etc.). Regarding the effect of proficiency, speech rate, use of language, and immersion on the patterns of pragmatic markers’ usage, a series of multiple regression analyses showed that L2 learners and heritage speakers are affected by them differently. For L2 learners, speech rate was significantly correlated with higher frequencies of pragmatic markers’ use. Concerning the range of pragmatic expressions used, proficiency, immersion, and speech rate had a significant effect. In contrast, the variable of use of Spanish was not a significant predictor. For heritage speakers, the analysis only revealed a significant correlation between the variable use of Spanish and the variety in pragmatic markers’ usage. In other words, those heritage speakers who reported using Spanish more frequently showed a broader range of pragmatic expressions. In contrast to L2 learners, proficiency, speech rate and participation in immersion experiences did not affect heritage speakers’ production of pragmatic markers. These findings suggest that the acquisition and use of pragmatic markers by heritage speakers and L2 learners are influenced by different variables, as well as shaped differently by their language practices. For L2 learners, proficiency, speech rate, and immersion experiences are significant predictors, but not for heritage speakers. In light of these results, I discuss how standardized tests traditionally used to measure proficiency do not reflect heritage speakers’ real oral command of Spanish, and how heritage speakers benefit from earlier and more naturalistic exposure to Spanish (vs. L2 learners). ; Limited ; Author requested closed access (OA after 2yrs) in Vireo ETD system
Keyword: Bilingualism; Heritage Language Acquisition; Heritage Speakers; L2 Learners; L2 Pragmatics; Pragmatic Markers; Pragmatics; Second Language Acquisition
URL: http://hdl.handle.net/2142/108311
BASE
Hide details
138
Training in machine translation post-editing for foreign language students
Zhang, Hong; Torres-Hostench, Olga. - : University of Hawaii National Foreign Language Resource Center, 2022. : Center for Language & Technology, 2022. : (co-sponsored by Center for Open Educational Resources and Language Learning, University of Texas at Austin), 2022
BASE
Show details
139
Tracking linguistic change in childhood: Transmission, incrementation, and vernacular reorganization
Holmes-Elliott, S; Smith, J. - : Linguistic Society of America, 2022
BASE
Show details
140
L2 discourse markers and the development of interactional competence during study abroad
Schirm, Ronald Samuel Karl. - : University of Waterloo, 2022
BASE
Show details

Page: 1...3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11...2.306

Catalogues
4.254
497
5.815
0
45
76
559
Bibliographies
29.931
11
1.166
0
56
31
128
126
458
Linked Open Data catalogues
0
Online resources
2.130
475
58
15
Open access documents
10.134
191
0
1
0
© 2013 - 2024 Lin|gu|is|tik | Imprint | Privacy Policy | Datenschutzeinstellungen ändern