DE eng

Search in the Catalogues and Directories

Page: 1 2
Hits 1 – 20 of 23

1
Content Word Production during Discourse in Aphasia: Deficits in Word Quantity, Not Lexical-Semantic Complexity. ...
Alyahya, Reem SW; Halai, Ajay D; Conroy, Paul. - : Apollo - University of Cambridge Repository, 2021
BASE
Show details
2
Content Word Production during Discourse in Aphasia: Deficits in Word Quantity, Not Lexical-Semantic Complexity.
Halai, Ajay D; Alyahya, Reem SW; Conroy, Paul. - : MIT Press - Journals, 2021. : Department of Psychiatry, 2021. : J Cogn Neurosci, 2021
BASE
Show details
3
A unified model of post-stroke language deficits including discourse production and their neural correlates. ...
Alyahya, Reem SW; Halai, Ajay; Conroy, Paul. - : Apollo - University of Cambridge Repository, 2020
BASE
Show details
4
A unified model of post-stroke language deficits including discourse production and their neural correlates.
Conroy, Paul; Halai, Ajay; Alyahya, Reem SW. - : Oxford University Press (OUP), 2020. : Brain, 2020
BASE
Show details
5
A unified model of post-stroke language deficits including discourse production and their neural correlates
In: Brain (2020)
BASE
Show details
6
Speech and language therapists’ perspectives of therapeutic alliance construction and maintenance in aphasia rehabilitation post‐stroke
Lawton, Michelle; Sage, Karen; Haddock, Gillian. - : John Wiley and Sons Inc., 2018
BASE
Show details
7
Noun and verb processing in aphasia: Behavioural profiles and neural correlates
BASE
Show details
8
ReaDySpeech for people with dysarthria after stroke: protocol for a feasibility randomised controlled trial ...
BASE
Show details
9
ReaDySpeech for people with dysarthria after stroke: protocol for a feasibility randomised controlled trial ...
BASE
Show details
10
Interventions for dysarthria due to stroke and other adult-acquired, non-progressive brain injury
BASE
Show details
11
The behavioural patterns and neural correlates of concrete and abstract verb processing in aphasia: A novel verb semantic battery
BASE
Show details
12
Interventions for dysarthria due to stroke and other adult‐acquired, non‐progressive brain injury
Abstract: BACKGROUND: Dysarthria is an acquired speech disorder following neurological injury that reduces intelligibility of speech due to weak, imprecise, slow and/or unco‐ordinated muscle control. The impact of dysarthria goes beyond communication and affects psychosocial functioning. This is an update of a review previously published in 2005. The scope has been broadened to include additional interventions, and the title amended accordingly. OBJECTIVES: To assess the effects of interventions to improve dysarthric speech following stroke and other non‐progressive adult‐acquired brain injury such as trauma, infection, tumour and surgery. SEARCH METHODS: We searched the Cochrane Stroke Group Trials Register (May 2016), CENTRAL (Cochrane Library 2016, Issue 4), MEDLINE, Embase, and CINAHL on 6 May 2016. We also searched Linguistics and Language Behavioral Abstracts (LLBA) (1976 to November 2016) and PsycINFO (1800 to September 2016). To identify further published, unpublished and ongoing trials, we searched major trials registers: WHO ICTRP, the ISRCTN registry, and ClinicalTrials.gov. We also handsearched the reference lists of relevant articles and contacted academic institutions and other researchers regarding other published, unpublished or ongoing trials. We did not impose any language restrictions. SELECTION CRITERIA: We selected randomised controlled trials (RCTs) comparing dysarthria interventions with 1) no intervention, 2) another intervention for dysarthria (this intervention may differ in methodology, timing of delivery, duration, frequency or theory), or 3) an attention control. DATA COLLECTION AND ANALYSIS: Three review authors selected trials for inclusion, extracted data, and assessed risk of bias. We attempted to contact study authors for clarification and missing data as required. We calculated standardised mean difference (SMD) and 95% confidence interval (CI), using a random‐effects model, and performed sensitivity analyses to assess the influence of methodological quality. We planned to conduct subgroup analyses for underlying clinical conditions. MAIN RESULTS: We included five small trials that randomised a total of 234 participants. Two studies were assessed as low risk of bias; none of the included studies were adequately powered. Two studies used an attention control and three studies compared to an alternative intervention, which in all cases was one intervention versus usual care intervention. The searches we carried out did not find any trials comparing an intervention with no intervention. The searches did not find any trials of an intervention that compared variations in timing, dose, or intensity of treatment using the same intervention. Four studies included only people with stroke; one included mostly people with stroke, but also those with brain injury. Three studies delivered interventions in the first few months after stroke; two recruited people with chronic dysarthria. Three studies evaluated behavioural interventions, one investigated acupuncture and another transcranial magnetic stimulation. One study included people with dysarthria within a broader trial of people with impaired communication. Our primary analysis of a persisting (three to nine months post‐intervention) effect at the activity level of measurement found no evidence in favour of dysarthria intervention compared with any control (SMD 0.18, 95% CI ‐0.18 to 0.55; 3 trials, 116 participants, GRADE: low quality, I² = 0%). Findings from sensitivity analysis of studies at low risk of bias were similar, with a slightly wider confidence interval and low heterogeneity (SMD 0.21, 95% CI ‐0.30 to 0.73, I² = 32%; 2 trials, 92 participants, GRADE: low quality). Subgroup analysis results for stroke were similar to the primary analysis because few non‐stroke participants had been recruited to trials (SMD 0.16, 95% CI ‐0.23 to 0.54, I² = 0%; 3 trials, 106 participants, GRADE: low quality). Similar results emerged from most of the secondary analyses. There was no evidence of a persisting effect at the impairment (SMD 0.07, 95% CI ‐0.91 to 1.06, I² = 70%; 2 trials, 56 participants, GRADE: very low quality) or participation level (SMD ‐0.11, 95% CI ‐0.56 to 0.33, I² = 0%; 2 trials, 79 participants, GRADE: low quality) but substantial heterogeneity on the former. Analyses of immediate post‐intervention outcomes provided no evidence of any short‐term benefit on activity (SMD 0.29, 95% CI ‐0.07 to 0.66, I² = 0%; 3 trials, 117 participants, GRADE: very low quality); or participation (SMD ‐0.24, 95% CI ‐0.94 to 0.45; 1 study, 32 participants) levels of measurement. There was a statistically significant effect favouring intervention at the immediate, impairment level of measurement (SMD 0.47, 95% CI 0.02 to 0.92, P = 0.04, I² = 0%; 4 trials, 99 participants, GRADE: very low quality) but only one of these four trials had a low risk of bias. AUTHORS' CONCLUSIONS: We found no definitive, adequately powered RCTs of interventions for people with dysarthria. We found limited evidence to suggest there may be an immediate beneficial effect on impairment level measures; more, higher quality research is needed to confirm this finding. Although we evaluated five studies, the benefits and risks of interventions remain unknown and the emerging evidence justifies the need for adequately powered clinical trials into this condition. People with dysarthria after stroke or brain injury should continue to receive rehabilitation according to clinical guidelines.
Keyword: Medicine General & Introductory Medical Sciences
URL: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28121021
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6464736/
https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD002088.pub3
BASE
Hide details
13
Case series, neuroscience-infused, computational neuropsychology will play a crucial role in the future of aphasiology : commentary on Laine and Martin, "Cognitive neuropsychology has been, is, and will be significant to aphasiology"
In: Aphasiology. - London [u.a.] : Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group 26 (2012) 11, 1381-1386
BLLDB
OLC Linguistik
Show details
14
Can impairment-focused therapy change the everyday conversations of people with aphasia? A review of the literature and future directions
In: Aphasiology. - London [u.a.] : Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group 26 (2012) 7, 895-916
BLLDB
OLC Linguistik
Show details
15
Errorless learning and rehabilitation of language and memory impairments
Lambon Ralph, Matthew A. (Hrsg.); Conroy, Paul (Hrsg.). - London [u.a.] : Routledge, 2012
BLLDB
UB Frankfurt Linguistik
Show details
16
The effects of decreasing and increasing cue therapy on improving naming speed and accuracy for verbs and nouns in aphasia
In: Aphasiology. - London [u.a.] : Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group 23 (2009) 6, 707-730
BLLDB
OLC Linguistik
Show details
17
Errorless and errorful therapy for verb and noun naming in aphasia
In: Aphasiology. - London [u.a.] : Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group 23 (2009) 11, 1311-1337
BLLDB
OLC Linguistik
Show details
18
A comparison of word versus sentence cues as therapy for verb naming in aphasia
In: Aphasiology. - London [u.a.] : Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group 23 (2009) 4, 462-482
OLC Linguistik
Show details
19
A comparison of word versus sentence cues as therapy for verb naming in aphasia
In: Aphasiology. - London [u.a.] : Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group 23 (2009) 4, 462-482
BLLDB
Show details
20
Improved vocabulary production after naming therapy in aphasia: can gains in picture naming generalise to connected speech?
In: International journal of language & communication disorders. - Oxford : Wiley-Blackwell 44 (2009) 6, 1036-1062
BLLDB
OLC Linguistik
Show details

Page: 1 2

Catalogues
1
0
8
0
0
0
0
Bibliographies
8
0
0
1
0
0
0
0
0
Linked Open Data catalogues
0
Online resources
0
0
0
0
Open access documents
12
0
0
0
0
© 2013 - 2024 Lin|gu|is|tik | Imprint | Privacy Policy | Datenschutzeinstellungen ändern